Display Bilingual:

Hey, I'm Sean, host of the Gray Area, 00:00
coming to you this week from a balcony 00:03
in sunny Denver, Colorado. For the show 00:06
this week, I talked to Charon Ranganoth. 00:09
He's a neuroscientist and author of the 00:11
book Why We Remember. It's one of my 00:14
favorite conversations we've had in a 00:17
while. It's a terrific book about how 00:19
memory works, why we remember what we 00:21
remember, and why we forget what we 00:23
forget, and how much of our memory is 00:25
actually just us making stuff up, and 00:28
how all of that shapes our identities as 00:31
individuals and also really as 00:34
societies. Anyway, it's terrific. I 00:37
think you'll love it. I did. And I'll 00:39
see you next week. 00:42
>> Dr. Shron Ragenoth, welcome to the show. 00:43
>> Thanks for having me, Sean. It's a 00:46
pleasure. Uh it's been too long since we 00:48
had a legit full-blown scientist on the 00:51
show. U 00:54
>> lots of pressure there. 00:56
>> Yeah, you got to carry the banner. Um 00:57
let's get into this wonderful book um 01:00
you wrote. I learned a lot. And 01:02
one of the things you say very early on 01:06
is that 01:09
the most important message to come out 01:11
of the whole science of memory isn't 01:13
that we should remember more, which is 01:16
what everyone just naturally wants to 01:18
do. You say that we actually have the 01:20
wrong expectation about what memory is 01:21
actually for. So I just wanted to start 01:25
there. What is memory for? 01:27
>> Well, I can tell you what it's not for. 01:30
And this is where the wrong expectation 01:32
comes in. It's not for storing and 01:34
hoarding every experience from the past. 01:36
It's about really giving us a resource 01:39
that we can draw upon to understand 01:42
what's happening right here and now and 01:44
to plan ahead and to anticipate what 01:47
could happen in the future. 01:49
>> So when 01:52
people say I have a bad memory, my 01:55
memory sucks, my memory is spotty. What 01:58
do you think they're actually 02:01
misunderstanding about how memory 02:03
functions? 02:05
>> Yeah, I mean it's a great question. I 02:06
have that voice in my head too that says 02:07
I have a terrible memory, right? But uh 02:10
I've been saying that for a long time 02:12
and I never struggled in school. Um I 02:14
could always remember things that I 02:18
needed to for work for instance. And it 02:22
wasn't so much that I had a bad memory 02:25
as much as I couldn't always remember 02:28
what I felt was important in the moment 02:31
when I needed it. And I think that's 02:33
what sometimes people feel. Uh but if 02:36
somebody really has a bad memory, like a 02:39
bad memory, they couldn't function in 02:42
life, they couldn't work, they couldn't 02:44
live independently. And I know cuz I've 02:46
I've tested people and I've I've 02:48
interviewed patients with bad really bad 02:50
clinically bad memories and they just 02:53
can't function. So I think often when I 02:55
see people complain about their memory, 02:58
they're often hyperfunctioning people 03:00
who just feel like they should be 03:03
remembering everything. And so that 03:05
that's what I'm talking about is I'm 03:07
really addressing those people. 03:09
So if a truly bad memory means you can't 03:11
function as an adult in the world, fair 03:16
enough. But clearly some memories are 03:19
better than others, right? Or or is the 03:21
mistake my mistake insisting on using 03:23
words like better or worse? 03:25
>> That's exactly the mistake is using 03:27
words like better or worse, right? So 03:29
it's often and I say that meaning that 03:31
people often conflate more with better. 03:35
So here here's a good example of this, 03:39
right? So there's people who have what's 03:40
called highly superior autobiographical 03:43
memory. So these are people who can go 03:45
back years into the past and recall from 03:47
a particular date like what they did for 03:51
what they had for breakfast or you know 03:54
who won a baseball game that day and 03:56
what the score was, the weather um that 03:58
day. Um all of these details that most 04:01
uh neurotypical people would not 04:05
remember, right? And so you'd think, 04:07
okay, these people have a great memory. 04:09
Uh but for one, they don't in the sense 04:12
that if you ask them to learn a new 04:14
language, they will probably learn at 04:18
the same speed as anyone who doesn't 04:20
have highly superior autobiographical 04:23
memory. Um based on the research that's 04:25
out there so far, there doesn't seem to 04:27
be some kind of a benefit that makes 04:30
them just better at everything. Um, the 04:32
second thing is we know if you look at 04:35
reports from people with highly superior 04:39
autobiographical memory, it's not like 04:41
they're like Bradley Cooper on 04:43
Limitless. They're not like uh 04:44
controlling the world. They're not any 04:47
more happy or successful than other 04:50
people as far as we can tell. And if 04:52
anything, they're a little bit plagued 04:54
by this memory because they often will 04:56
ruminate about things that happened, 04:59
even very minor things that happened 05:02
that were negative. And so, is that a 05:03
better memory? I mean, some people 05:06
online I've seen have described it as a 05:07
curse that they wouldn't wish on anyone. 05:10
>> Yeah. I mean, I don't want to be, you 05:12
know, Rainman or or Bradley Cooper here. 05:15
Um, but I'm sure I'm not alone. 05:17
>> Well, I wouldn't be I wouldn't mind 05:20
being Bradley Cooper for a day, but 05:21
I'm sure he has his own issues. 05:24
Everybody does, right? 05:26
>> Fair. Um, it would be fun to be more 05:26
handsome for a day. But sorry about 05:29
that. Uh my question would be why is it 05:31
that I can have near photographic 05:34
memories about certain things uh certain 05:38
books, certain events, other mostly 05:41
random stupid trivial stuff and have so 05:44
little capacity so often to remember 05:47
things like faces and names even though 05:51
I'd very much like to remember faces and 05:53
names. And I think there are very good 05:56
reasons, moral, uh, social, whatever, to 05:59
remember faces and names. And yet I just 06:02
consistently 06:06
botch it. U, why is that? 06:07
>> Well, there's a whole lot of reasons. 06:11
But, um, if we, uh, talk about the 06:13
problem of faces and names, this is 06:16
something that everyone who complains 06:17
about memory, this is the first thing 06:20
that comes up. So, you're not alone. 06:22
Essentially, when we're trying to pull 06:24
information from memory, we have a 06:26
challenge which is that memories compete 06:29
with each other. So any experience that 06:31
we have uh there's all sorts of 06:34
competition for it. And just as an 06:37
analogy, you know, my desk right now, 06:39
I'm trying to gather information for my 06:40
taxes cuz I postpone them and I spend a 06:42
good amount of time trying to find 06:45
things all the time, right? And so 06:46
memory can be thought of in a very 06:49
similar way that when you have a lot of 06:51
similar things cluttered around and not 06:53
organized, you're going to have a lot of 06:56
competition, a lot of trouble finding 06:58
it. But if there was one thing that 07:00
truly stood out here, like if I had a 07:03
violin amidst all these other things, I 07:05
would be able to find that pretty 07:08
easily, right? Um, so here's the problem 07:09
now getting back to faces and names is 07:12
we hear many many similar names. 07:15
Sometimes the same name attached to 07:18
multiple faces, right? And faces are 07:20
exceptionally similar to one another. 07:23
There's obviously your face is different 07:26
from mine, but we both have two eyes, we 07:28
have a nose, we have mouths. And even 07:31
amongst, you know, people we know well, 07:35
there's often a lot of similarity. Uh so 07:38
now we have a real problem is that we 07:41
have a bunch of faces that kind of are 07:42
similar to each other. There's a number 07:44
of names that are similar to one another 07:46
that we've seen in the past. And the 07:49
association between a name and a face is 07:51
just utterly ridiculous, right? I mean, 07:53
how do you there's nothing meaningful 07:56
linking a name to a face. So if you met 07:58
somebody whose name was Baker, you know, 08:01
hundreds of years ago, that would be 08:03
easier because at least they were 08:05
probably baking bread and you know, or 08:07
Smith would be the guy who's making your 08:10
suit of armor or your swords or 08:11
whatever, right? And so now there's just 08:13
no link between names and faces. There's 08:15
nothing meaningful there. So that's why 08:18
it's so hard. 08:20
>> How differently does memory function at 08:22
different stages of life? I think we all 08:26
understand that memory declines 08:29
uh as we get older, but there are 08:32
certain kinds of memory that seem to 08:34
hold up better than others as we age, 08:36
right? What is the reason for that? 08:39
>> Let's get into the different kinds of 08:42
memory first of all. So, um there's many 08:43
different kinds of memory. memory 08:46
researchers love to slice them and dice 08:48
them in different ways, but one that I 08:49
think is meaningful, especially when we 08:53
talk about development, is the 08:54
distinction between what's called 08:56
semantic memory and episodic memory. And 08:57
so semantic memory might be like, you 09:01
know, remembering uh something about, 09:04
let's say, like uh um you know, a 09:07
history lesson that you took when you 09:09
were in high school and remembering the 09:11
facts about World War II or being able 09:13
to build knowledge about like uh 09:16
computers or something like that and 09:20
that kind of expertise that you can 09:21
build up. Uh, but episodic memory is the 09:23
ability to link something to one 09:26
particular moment in time, like a movie 09:28
that you saw about World War II when you 09:31
were on a strange date when you were in 09:33
college or something and for some reason 09:37
you decided to see an old showing of 09:38
Midway or whatever it is, right? Um, and 09:40
those particular events that are unique 09:43
to a place in a time are called episodic 09:46
memories. And so when people say their 09:49
memory gets worse as they get older, 09:52
they're almost always talking about 09:54
episodic memory, which is that uh people 09:56
when you get older, you find yourself 10:00
more forgetful, misplacing things and 10:03
not being able to remember where they 10:05
were or not being able to remember faces 10:07
and names. Semantic memory actually 10:10
remains solid and even kind of improves 10:13
a little bit as people get older. um 10:15
that knowledge about the world. It's not 10:18
like when you're 70, you lose knowledge 10:20
about all the things that you've 10:23
learned, all that wisdom, so to speak, 10:25
right? So, what you do lose though is 10:27
the ability to pull up those facts 10:30
sometimes. And so, you'll end up being 10:33
like, I know the name of that actor, but 10:35
I can't get it. It's that guy who's in 10:38
that thing. And that happens a lot, too. 10:41
And that's not a issue of losing the 10:43
memory or not being able to form 10:45
semantic memories as much as it is a 10:47
problem of not being able to pull up 10:49
that information. And that has to do 10:52
with something else altogether which we 10:54
call executive function. And in fact, 10:56
that's actually probably one of the 10:59
biggest reasons why memory gets episodic 11:01
memory gets worse as you get older is 11:03
you lose that ability to regulate your 11:06
attention, that ability to pull up 11:08
strategies, that ability to kind of like 11:10
not just make memories, but make 11:13
memories useful. And that's related to 11:15
the functions of an area called the 11:18
prefrontal cortex. And the prefrontal 11:20
cortex's function starts to decline 11:22
pretty much from the age of 30. And it's 11:24
a gradual decline in most people that 11:26
continues throughout the lifespan. 11:29
>> I'm on the wrong side of that. 11:31
>> Yeah, me too. I always tell people the 11:33
biggest memory hack I can give is to 11:36
tell people what do you want? Tell ask 11:39
yourself what do I want to remember in 11:42
the first place? What's the important 11:44
thing that I need to take away from 11:45
this? If you came in to a cocktail party 11:47
and you knew you wanted to be a good 11:50
person and meet all, you know, get to 11:53
know the names of all the people you 11:55
meet and remember them, then start with 11:56
that intention. Most people think that 11:59
they'll get the memories for free and 12:01
that's just wrong. There are studies 12:03
showing that the happiness and 12:06
satisfaction we get from the outcomes of 12:10
our decisions do not come as you might 12:14
expect from what we actually experienced 12:19
but rather it comes from what we 12:22
remember. 12:24
That seems like a pretty significant 12:26
finding. What is the explanation for 12:28
that? 12:31
Well, memory really is so transient. And 12:32
what I mean by this is is that we don't 12:37
forget everything, right? But we forget 12:39
many of the details very, very quickly. 12:41
Some of the earliest studies of memory 12:44
showed that in fact within about one 12:46
hour of memorizing things that you would 12:50
lose about 60% of what you tried to 12:53
memorize. 12:56
>> Now, think about that. That's like 60% 12:56
of the details of your experiencing self 12:59
at this moment gone in an hour right 13:03
now. That doesn't mean that you don't 13:07
remember anything. But uh Danny Conaman 13:08
who won the Nobel Prize in economics, he 13:11
was actually said a lot of smart things 13:14
about memory. Like he just said smart 13:16
things about a lot of things, but one 13:18
was memory where he said people 13:19
generally remember the beginnings and 13:21
the endings and the highs and the lows. 13:23
And that in some ways is the important 13:25
stuff that you need for making a 13:28
decision, not necessarily every bit of 13:30
experience. 13:34
>> One takeaway for me here is is that the 13:34
stories we tell ourselves u the the 13:38
narratives that make up our memory 13:41
really matter in terms of our 13:43
well-being. And that would seem to mean 13:47
that we could gain more happiness in 13:49
life by deliberately focusing on the 13:52
right things by trying to remember the 13:56
right things. Um 13:59
>> I think that was a beautiful way of 14:00
putting it. Um I like to say that memory 14:02
is more like a painting than a 14:04
photograph. You know, there's a famous 14:06
painting by Van Go of this uh French 14:08
church, and I like to show this 14:10
sometimes when I'm talking to people uh 14:12
and people will immediately recognize 14:14
the painting as one of the church, 14:16
right? But it by no means is exactly 14:18
like the church that's there. I mean, 14:22
there's a lot of stuff that's could be 14:24
thought of as missing or distorted 14:26
relative to what's in the church itself, 14:28
what what it looks like, but there's a 14:30
lot that's just his perspective that's 14:32
in there. And that's what makes a 14:34
painting beautiful in some ways is stuff 14:36
that's neither accurate nor inaccurate, 14:38
but it's just how the person sees the 14:41
world. He painted it at night. You could 14:42
have just as easily painted it in the 14:44
morning. You could paint it from a 14:45
different point of view. You could paint 14:48
it small. You could paint it big. You 14:50
could look at one door, you know? And 14:52
every time we remember an event, we're 14:55
creating a new painting, right? So often 14:57
people will tell me about good and bad 15:01
memories, good and bad experiences, but 15:03
that can change. And I think even though 15:06
that seems a little bit 15:09
counterintuitive, I think many of us 15:10
have had times in our life which were 15:12
objectively 15:15
bad, sometimes terrible, and we can look 15:17
back on that from a completely different 15:19
point of view. So in my book I talk 15:21
about an experience which just like 15:23
almost everything that could go wrong 15:26
did go wrong when I was going on this 15:27
paddle boarding expedition 15:29
almost died you know in that thing and 15:32
now it's a pretty funny story I really 15:35
like to share it. Yeah. Well that that 15:37
story actually in the book made me think 15:39
about 15:42
trauma and memory and even worse kinds 15:44
of traumatic experiences than 15:47
>> than that. Right. Right. I mean, so for 15:49
someone who's been through uh real 15:51
trauma, uh just terrible, terrible 15:54
trauma, forgetting 15:56
can feel impossible. I mean, how do 15:59
traumatic memories differ from ordinary 16:01
memories in the brain? Do they differ at 16:05
all? 16:07
>> Oh, they do for sure. Um for one thing, 16:07
the brain is wired in a way to 16:10
preferentially keep memories for things 16:13
that are emotionally significant. And 16:16
the reason is our emotions are tied to 16:19
they're not equivalent to but they're 16:22
tied to these motivational circuits in 16:23
our brain that are trying to keep us 16:25
alive, right? And so um often when 16:28
people have a traumatic event, they feel 16:32
an exist existential threat and they're 16:34
often arousing or stressful. And there's 16:37
chemicals in our brain that are released 16:40
during these experiences. 16:42
Noradrenaline's one. Um cortisol is a 16:43
stress hormone. There's gobs of hormones 16:47
that are released during stress. Uh, but 16:48
there's also exciting kind of more 16:51
positive experiences like that kind of 16:53
moment when you're going to get your 16:55
first kiss or something like that and 16:58
that's associated with dopamine release 16:59
in the brain. There's all sorts of 17:01
chemicals like this. They promote 17:02
plasticity. They allow new memories to 17:04
be locked in much more effectively, 17:08
right? So, that's why these traumatic 17:10
memories tend to stick with us often. 17:13
Um, but what's interesting about it is 17:16
we've studied some stressful memories 17:19
and some emotionally significant 17:21
memories, not in ways that are remotely 17:23
like something that a trauma 17:25
survivor really experiences. But what we 17:28
find even with those is there's a 17:31
difference between the brain systems 17:34
that actually um are involved in giving 17:36
you the details of what happened and the 17:40
brain systems that actually just rev you 17:42
up and give you the visceral feeling of 17:45
the experience, right? So let's say you 17:47
got into a car accident. Later on you 17:50
remember the car accident. Well, there's 17:51
the details of what happened, right? 17:53
like this person ran a stop sign and 17:55
they crashed right into me and they're 17:57
driving like a red car. Um, and then 17:59
there's the feeling the feeling of 18:02
imminent danger, the feeling of pain 18:04
when you got hit and you you jerked 18:06
forward or something, right? And it 18:09
turns out that those are different, but 18:11
the thing that makes people feel like 18:14
it's a photographic memory or they can 18:16
experience it like in the moment is not 18:18
the details. It's really the emotion 18:21
that's associated with it. And so that's 18:24
a key thing that makes traumatic 18:25
memories different. And this brings me 18:27
to another point that I I like to bring 18:29
up to people who tell me, you know, can 18:31
I can you help me forget this 18:33
experience? You want to be able to 18:35
remember them without that emotional 18:37
punch in the face. Lots of people who do 18:40
survive traumas can do that. Um the 18:43
brain has natural ways. Matt Walker says 18:46
that it happens during sleep. And I 18:49
think that is a part of it that the 18:52
brain changes these memories to become 18:54
more useful to us and less toxic to us 18:57
later on, less radioactive. 19:01
But when it doesn't happen, that's when 19:04
people get PTSD. I 19:06
really enjoyed the bits in the book 19:10
about music and the unique power it has. 19:11
And and I thought I thought it was 19:15
fascinating when you were talking about 19:17
the study you worked on, I think when 19:20
you were uh just a you know a baby grad 19:23
student. Um and and you you expected 19:26
that having subjects listen to sad music 19:29
would make it easier for them to 19:33
remember sad events in their lives and 19:35
and that remembering sad events would 19:38
make them sad. But why does listening to 19:40
sad music make us remember sad events 19:42
and does the dynamic also work in the 19:46
opposite direction? I think this is a 19:48
very important point is that our 19:50
episodic memories are rooted to a place 19:51
in time and part of what makes a unique 19:55
time in our lives, a unique moment in 19:59
time significant is uh the feelings that 20:02
we have in the time. You might be 20:06
remembering something like, you know, 20:08
your high school graduation and there's 20:10
the content of that memory, but then 20:12
there's also the feelings that you had 20:15
at that moment. And that those feelings 20:17
are part of what we call the context. 20:20
And so if you imagine episodic memories, 20:22
if they were books in a library, you 20:25
would be sorting those books not 20:28
according to the content, but really to 20:30
the context. 20:32
And that as I said the emotions are part 20:34
of that context. And so if in the moment 20:37
you are in a particular emotional state 20:41
it makes it's almost like as if you're 20:44
looking at the section of the library 20:46
that has information relevant to that 20:49
context. And so uh that's why music can 20:52
be such a powerful vehicle. And it's not 20:55
only the emotional part. It also can be 20:57
because you listen to particular kinds 21:00
of music during particular times in your 21:02
life. 21:04
>> Yeah, that's it's such a unique trigger 21:05
in that way. I think you I mean you talk 21:08
in the book about, you know, if you hear 21:09
a couple of chords of born in the USA, 21:12
it reminds you of some of the racist 21:15
you would hear when you were in 21:17
like junior high or high school. And you 21:20
know, it it made me think if I hear the 21:23
the Rolling Stone song Wild Horses, it 21:25
takes me right back to when my mom died 21:28
in 2020. I listen to that song over and 21:30
over again uh when she passed away 21:32
because it reminded me of her. It was 21:35
her favorite song. And for the rest of 21:37
my life, if that song comes on, I am 21:38
right back there when that happened. I 21:41
mean, it's just I just there's nothing 21:43
else that quite does that in that way. I 21:45
think part of it is the emotional pull 21:49
and part of it is the uniqueness of 21:51
particular kinds of music or pieces of 21:54
music to particular kinds of 21:56
relationships we have or times in our 21:59
life. Who we are is constantly changing, 22:01
right? And music is often tied to our 22:04
sense of who we are that it's like we 22:08
choose particular times of music based 22:10
on who we are at a particular time in 22:12
our life. And so music is this beautiful 22:13
vehicle to access those memories of who 22:16
we were. And that's part of I think why 22:20
there's this nostalgia that's associated 22:24
with music is because it really takes us 22:26
back not only to the memories of what 22:28
happened, but the feeling of what we 22:31
were like when we were at that point in 22:33
our lives. 22:35
>> Support for the show comes from Bombas. 22:37
You know those songs that make you just 22:39
want to run? Maybe you're in the middle 22:41
of a workout and you're totally spent. 22:43
But then that one track hits, maybe it's 22:46
tulle, maybe it's a little wooang and 22:48
boom, you're back. That's kind of like 22:50
what Bombas socks do. You put them on 22:52
and you're ready to go. This summer, you 22:55
can get moving with Bombas socks. 22:57
Specially designed to wick sweat, keep 22:59
you cool, and prevent blisters. You 23:01
probably know I've tried out Bombas for 23:03
myself. They are my favorite socks. In 23:05
the winter and fall, I rock their wool 23:08
socks because I wear boots a lot and 23:10
they're super thick but still 23:12
breathable. Now that it's 500° down here 23:14
on the Gulf Coast, my go-to socks are 23:18
their lightweight athletic socks. They 23:20
are comfortable and they really do keep 23:23
you cool and dry. You can head over to 23:25
bombas.com/gra 23:27
area and use code grey area for 20% off 23:30
your first purchase. That's 23:33
boombas.com/gray 23:34
area codegray area at checkout. 23:37
Support for the show comes from Shopify. 23:45
When you're starting a new business, it 23:48
can feel like you're expected to do it 23:50
all. Marketing, design, and everything 23:51
in between, even if you've never done 23:54
half of it before. What you really need 23:56
is a tool that helps you reach your 23:58
goals without having to master every 24:00
skill yourself. For millions of 24:02
businesses, that tool is Shopify. Their 24:04
design studio lets you build a beautiful 24:07
online store that matches your brand 24:10
style, letting you choose from hundreds 24:12
of readytouse templates. You can also 24:14
set up your content creation by using 24:16
their helpful host of AI tools. And you 24:18
can even create email and social media 24:21
campaigns with ease and meet your 24:23
customers wherever they're scrolling or 24:25
strolling. See why Shopify is the 24:27
commerce platform behind millions of 24:29
businesses around the world. If you're 24:31
ready to sell, you can be ready with 24:33
Shopify. You can turn your big business 24:35
idea into a reality with Shopify on your 24:38
side. You can sign up for your $1 per 24:40
month trial period and start selling 24:43
today at shopify.com/box. 24:45
Go to shopify.com/box. 24:48
Shopify.com/box. 24:51
I wanted to ask you um about the role of 24:58
memory in shaping our identities, our 25:02
our sense of self. And you talk about 25:05
how our life narrative is really this 25:07
thing stitched together through memory 25:11
and our social interactions and the 25:14
stories we tell about who we are and 25:18
what we are. My question is why do we 25:22
need these stories in the first place? I 25:25
mean is the self or whatever we call the 25:28
self just a memory system holding a 25:31
story together? 25:35
>> I in some ways yes I think so. In fact 25:37
there are people who talk in my field 25:40
about the self-memory system. Uh, I in 25:43
the book, if there's one thing I'm 25:47
guilty of is avoiding some of the hard 25:49
questions, questions about consciousness 25:52
and the self. 25:54
>> Smart man. 25:55
>> Yeah, that was just a little bit of a 25:56
copout. I totally admit that. Um, but I 25:58
think you're on to something and I think 26:01
that it's like uh we have so I talk in 26:03
the book about schemas as a very 26:07
powerful way in which we can learn. So 26:09
schemas for instance could be something 26:12
as simple as I know what happens during 26:14
a basketball game. I know what happens 26:16
during a wedding. So these particular 26:18
kinds of events we have blueprints in 26:20
our head about things that are almost 26:22
certain to happen, things that could 26:25
happen and things that won't happen, 26:27
right? And that helps us orient 26:30
ourselves towards the unique aspects of 26:33
what um you're trying to remember at any 26:35
given time. uh but we also have those 26:38
ideas about oursel called a self schema. 26:40
And I think this is important because a 26:44
big part of imagination and planning is 26:46
understanding a little bit about who we 26:51
are and why we do things. We don't just 26:53
remember the past in terms of like 26:57
regurgitating a bunch of details. When 26:59
we're really remembering events, we 27:01
stitch it into causes and effects. And 27:04
that's really important because if 27:07
something is not a cause or an effect, 27:09
it's kind of window dressing, right? And 27:12
so we don't remember that somebody was 27:15
like, you know, left the room, but we 27:19
remember they left a room because they 27:22
were angry at us or because they were in 27:24
they just got interrupted by an 27:27
emergency phone call. And I think our 27:29
sense of who we are gives us a way of 27:32
organizing our past and organizing 27:36
things into causes and effects, too. 27:39
>> This is more of a philosophical question 27:41
than a scientific one, but this is the 27:44
show, so I'm going to ask anyway. Um, 27:48
if someone loses their memories, 27:52
if they cannot remember their life, 27:55
can they still be themselves? Can they 27:58
be the same self they were? 28:00
>> There's a uh there's a person named Stan 28:04
Klene at at UC Santa Barbara who asked 28:08
this very question and he makes a strong 28:10
case. I tend to think of philosophy as 28:12
above my pay grade. 28:14
There's a, you know, I tend to invert 28:17
the usual like kind of tech bro view of 28:19
the world and I kind of think of math 28:22
and physics as easy fields because 28:23
there's an answer and psychology is a 28:25
much more of a hard science because 28:27
there's not really clear answers and 28:29
philosophy is like where you get into 28:31
the stratosphere. 28:33
>> We just have to sound like we know what 28:34
we're talking about. You you people, the 28:36
scientists actually have to know what 28:38
>> Yeah. Yeah. Exactly. I think it's like 28:40
the real experts will say, "I don't 28:41
know." Um, and I don't know, but uh, uh, 28:43
Stan has studied people with very, very 28:46
dense amnesia and has made a good case 28:48
that these people still have a sense of 28:51
who they are. 28:53
>> But here's the thing. So there there's 28:55
not cases literally of people losing 28:57
every memory of what they've 29:00
experienced. But there are people, for 29:02
instance, like a patient named Casey 29:04
that Endel Tolving studied. He's he's a 29:06
really f he came up with the concept of 29:08
episodic memory. And the claim was he 29:10
could look all the way back in the past 29:13
and he still didn't have that ability to 29:14
mentally time travel, right? And there's 29:16
a little bit of an emptiness to them, a 29:19
little bit of a flatness. They're in the 29:22
moment, but there's not a sense of what 29:23
they could do or what they could be 29:27
because they don't have these episodic 29:31
memories to draw upon to give them like 29:33
kind of more of a flavor of who they 29:36
are. Um, and I think that's really 29:39
important because and like I said, our 29:42
sense of self normally is constantly 29:44
changing as we keep acquiring episodic 29:47
memories. 29:50
>> But people who have amnesia, even if 29:51
it's like something more like the 29:54
typical kind of amnesia is you have a 29:56
stroke or something and you're 29:58
unconscious for a while. when you come 30:00
to you lose memories for something. You 30:02
lose your ability to form new memories 30:05
and you might lose memories going back 30:06
10 years but you still remember your 30:08
childhood. And what happens is these 30:10
patients who have severe amnesia 30:12
will get stuck in time. You know, 30:15
they'll look in the mirror and they'll 30:18
say, "Oh my gosh, who is this person?" 30:19
But to them, it's like they're still 30:22
they still feel like they were the 30:24
person they were, you know, 20 years 30:26
ago, 30 years ago. And in some cases, I 30:28
think that's why people with severe 30:31
Alzheimer's, you sense that they're 30:33
regressing as they kind of like lose 30:35
that ability to go back to that 30:38
wellspring of episodic memories. So I 30:40
think yeah, we have a capability of 30:42
being having a self, but that self 30:44
becomes much emptier and thinner as you 30:46
lose memories. 30:50
>> Can we say that political movements in 30:52
some sense are doing the same thing just 30:55
at a 30:58
at another level. Are they stitching 31:00
collective memory into a collective 31:04
identity? 100% 100%. Uh this idea of 31:07
collective memory uh is uh still one 31:12
that we're is it's still a developing 31:15
concept in our field. A lot of the work 31:18
that's been done in collective memory is 31:20
actually done in the fields of history 31:22
and sociology. But it's a very relevant 31:24
concept. I mean just as an example 31:27
>> how would you define that if you don't 31:29
mind me asking just 31:30
>> absolutely I mean you can define it very 31:32
broadly which is a shared memory that a 31:33
memory that's shared amongst multiple 31:36
people if you have close family members 31:38
you probably have a collective memory of 31:40
the family and that gives you a sense of 31:42
your familial identity uh because you 31:45
all have this shared story right and 31:48
your experience might be different than 31:51
the experience of your parents or 31:52
siblings but once you share those 31:54
experiences with each other. It's no 31:56
longer your memory. Uh just the act of 31:58
expressing that memory in a way that 32:02
someone else can understand it changes 32:04
the memory for you. And then now the 32:05
other person has a memory for something 32:08
they've never experienced before because 32:10
you've told them about it. And so as 32:12
these memories go get passed back and 32:15
forth, they're constantly modified, 32:18
right? And I think this is key to what 32:20
makes humans so successful in the world 32:22
is that we can build memories and 32:25
benefit from other people's experiences 32:28
and and you know wisdom. But it creates 32:31
huge problems because memory can be 32:34
selective and memory can be you know so 32:36
it can be highly incomplete and it can 32:39
also be inaccurate many times. Right. 32:42
And so 32:45
>> sometimes intentionally. So 32:46
>> sometimes intentionally. Exactly. So if 32:47
you want to form an authoritarian 32:49
regime, the first thing you do is you 32:52
seize collective memory. You take down 32:55
statues. You start to regulate, you 32:58
know, the teaching of history in 33:02
schools. You remove references in 33:03
historical archives, right? Almost any 33:07
authoritarian regime. This is part of 33:10
the playbook. And uh and I think it's a 33:13
powerful one because once you start to 33:16
shape that narrative, just like people 33:19
have narratives of I'm terrible at math 33:21
or I'm like a failure in life and those 33:24
are often rooted in a particular 33:27
interpretation of your past. If you can 33:29
enforce most people to have a particular 33:32
view of their nation's past, that's 33:35
going to shape their sense of what it 33:37
means to be a member of that nation or 33:39
that society. 33:40
The uh relationship between individual 33:42
memory and collective memory or between 33:46
individual identity and collective 33:48
identity is so fascinating to me. I how 33:50
much of our sense of self is determined 33:55
or or shaped by our social 33:58
or tribal affiliations. I imagine the 34:03
relationship here is pretty damn 34:06
complicated, but there has to be a 34:07
relationship there. 34:11
>> There's absolutely a relationship and 34:12
it's a two-way relationship. 34:14
>> So, a lot of the research shows that on 34:16
the one hand, you develop your sense of 34:19
of uh tribal affiliations 34:23
or familial relationships through these 34:26
kind of collective memories that you 34:30
have. but also you're able to connect 34:31
with other people if you have shared 34:35
collective memories and you view those 34:37
memories in a similar light. And so what 34:40
I mean by that is, you know, if you're 34:42
in England and you support say the 34:43
Tottenham Spurs or something like that, 34:46
you can meet someone anywhere and once 34:48
you start talking about the same game, 34:51
they will become friends, right? Uh here 34:53
in the US it would be like an American 34:56
football team or a baseball team, same 34:58
thing, right? or let's say you're both 35:00
you both have similar political views 35:02
and you both like uh saw the same debate 35:05
or something like that. Just sharing 35:08
those collective memories can make you 35:10
feel more connected with that person. 35:12
>> Oh, I I grew up in the in the South. 35:15
That's where I am now. And you know, 35:17
like college football is sort of our 35:19
thing. It's a religion. 35:21
>> But it is people who who 35:22
>> didn't grow up in that don't quite 35:25
understand it at this level. what kind 35:27
of cultural shared cultural language it 35:30
is. I mean, it is it is that shared 35:33
memory really is you meet someone, they 35:36
got on an LSU Tigers shirt and before 35:39
you know it, you're having a 35:40
conversation about, oh no, I went there 35:41
when I was five. the Auburn game. Could 35:42
you believe, you know, I mean, it was 35:44
>> it it it has this way of of stitching 35:45
together a history and a community in a 35:47
way that that very few things can, but 35:50
it is that 35:52
>> it is that that shared cultural language 35:54
and history that's like the container 35:56
for the community really. 35:58
>> Yeah. It's beautiful in many ways and 36:00
it's toxic in many ways, right? It's 36:03
like 36:05
>> that's where I was going next. 36:06
>> Yeah. Yeah. I mean, it's both because 36:07
you see someone else with an LSU Tigers 36:09
thing shirt and now you feel like you've 36:11
got a bond with them and that's a really 36:15
good thing. But then you're both 36:16
watching an LSU Tigers game and you talk 36:18
to each other and you'll remember the 36:21
game based on your love for the LSU 36:23
Tigers and you'll go, "Yeah, the ref 36:26
totally blew that call." And you might 36:29
forget a really good play that was made 36:31
by the other team. and the other person 36:33
reinforces that story and you build a 36:35
collective memory that's more selective 36:37
and more distorted than it would be had 36:41
you never talked to each other. And 36:43
that's the downside of collective memory 36:45
is if we're not careful, if we don't 36:47
surround ourselves by a lot of checks 36:49
and balances, what happens is we can 36:51
develop very impoverished and inaccurate 36:54
collective memories and we're very 36:58
susceptible to misinformation because we 36:59
like misinformation if it comes in a 37:02
flavor that we already like and we're 37:04
much more likely to take it in and 37:07
digest it. 37:09
>> Yeah. Now, this is something 37:10
This is the challenge or one of the 37:15
challenge uh that comes from the fact 37:16
that memory it itself is so constructed 37:19
and slippery in this way. I mean you 37:23
even in the book you you talk a bit 37:24
about how as we grow more segregated 37:26
politically, culturally, 37:31
we end up with just totally different 37:34
memories 37:36
>> of the same events 37:37
>> and therefore different realities. 37:38
>> And if if memory is reconstructive in in 37:41
in the ways you're talking about and we 37:44
are moving more and more into a world 37:47
that's segregatedformationally. 37:50
Does that seem like a massive massive 37:53
political problem to you? I mean, I've 37:56
argued that it's just straight up 37:58
unsustainable, but it's also possible 38:00
that I'm being dramatic. Uh, so I put it 38:02
to you. I think this is one of the 38:05
biggest threats we have in the present 38:07
day and one of the biggest dangers that 38:09
I foresee as you know technologies like 38:12
AI become more and more powerful is you 38:15
know and maybe I'm I'm looking at this 38:18
from too much of a personal lens and too 38:21
much of a you know often we kind of look 38:23
at things like now is somehow special 38:25
right but I do feel like I've never 38:28
lived in a time where it's so easy for 38:30
people to just believe what makes them 38:34
happy, right? We used to all have shared 38:37
news sources and shared sources of 38:41
information and expertise that, you 38:44
know, they could tell us things that we 38:47
didn't like, like smoking is unhealthy. 38:48
And even if we didn't like that, we sort 38:51
of had to believe it because we were 38:54
all, you know, because we had news 38:56
sources that fact checked and we had 38:59
like uh uh experts who we trusted and 39:01
experts aren't always right, you know, 39:06
and news sources are definitely not 39:08
always right. And you know, news can be 39:09
a selective win uh lens on on collective 39:11
memory. But now we have so many uh 39:14
sources of information and the uh thing 39:19
that I of you know I've done a lot of 39:23
podcasts now and it's been a fascinating 39:25
experience for me because I hear you 39:27
know I remember going on one podcast and 39:29
and the person was talking about the 39:31
legacy media is dead and and they're 39:33
trying to go after podcasters because 39:36
podcasters are telling it like it is and 39:38
giving people these long form things. 39:40
And the idea is somehow if we hear 39:42
somebody talking for an hour and we just 39:44
let them say whatever they want that 39:46
eventually people will figure out the 39:49
truth, right? But that's not really how 39:51
it works. What happens is people pick 39:53
and choose what they will remember from 39:55
all of the podcasts they've listened to 39:58
>> based on what they believe. That's how 40:00
they're going to form that memory for 40:04
all those things that they've been 40:05
exposed to. And the more we allow 40:07
misinformation to thrive, 40:10
the more it likely it is that people can 40:13
just pick and choose what they get in a 40:17
way that makes them feel good. I brought 40:21
up this idea of causes as being really 40:23
important, causes and effects. And so we 40:26
often will decide about facts and we'll 40:29
make decisions about things in the world 40:34
based on our understanding of causes and 40:36
effects. But the way our brains do it is 40:38
kind of like making the best of bad 40:42
information. So for instance, it's like 40:45
if I were to hear about somebody who 40:47
gets a shot and they, you know, gets a 40:52
vaccination and they developed a severe 40:55
allergic reaction to it and they go into 40:58
aniflaxis, I will be more likely to 41:00
think that that per that vaccine causes 41:03
problems and is dangerous. 41:07
But I'm not hearing the stories about a 41:10
bunch of people who took the vaccine and 41:12
didn't get sick. I'm not hearing them 41:14
and because that's not an event. That's 41:17
not something that you can form a memory 41:19
for. It's something that didn't happen. 41:21
You know, hearing a statistic that you 41:22
know nine out of 10 people who took the 41:24
shot didn't get CO or whatever. That's 41:26
not really going to produce a vivid 41:29
memory that's going to influence you 41:31
nearly as much as this vivid memory of 41:33
somebody you even if it wasn't you just 41:35
somebody telling a story about this. 41:38
>> Do we understand how malleable 41:40
collective memory is? Is this something 41:43
that can evolve pretty quickly? If I can 41:45
tell myself a new story about myself 41:47
that transforms how I 41:50
>> experience the world or think about the 41:52
world and if you can do that as an 41:55
individual, 41:57
can a country do that? Can a community 41:59
do that? 42:01
Everything we know about collective 42:03
memory, which is not a lot, but 42:05
everything we know has really pointed to 42:08
the idea that collective memory just 42:11
magnifies the strengths and the 42:14
weaknesses of individual memory. And so 42:16
what I mean by that is we know that I 42:19
can give people misinformation after an 42:21
event has occurred. So you you know 42:24
Elizabeth Loftess did some really 42:26
beautiful work on this um where she 42:28
would show you know a slideshow of this 42:30
car accident and then the car accident 42:33
like somebody might have run through 42:36
like a stop sign and she'll say how fast 42:38
was it going when it passed the yield 42:41
sign and later on people might be more 42:42
likely to remember that there was a 42:45
yield sign there instead of a stop sign. 42:47
Right? Those are effects that you can 42:48
find in the lab for fairly mundane kind 42:51
of pieces of information. But now you're 42:53
in a group of people and you're 42:56
remembering that and one person 42:57
remembers something wrong, especially if 42:58
that person has kind of a position of 43:01
status. They're a little bit higher 43:03
status and they talk more. Um, which we 43:06
can all think of what kinds of groups 43:09
tend to do this, right? You know, 43:10
mansplating things and so forth. um 43:12
those people will have an undue 43:16
influence on how people remember the 43:18
event later on and that misinformation 43:20
can really thrive like a I mean they 43:22
talk about it as a social contagion 43:25
because it spreads like a virus. Um so 43:27
that selectivity and that malleability 43:30
of memory that we have as individuals 43:33
even fairly mundane experiences tends to 43:35
just magnify in groups. Uh but of course 43:38
you also have the power of belief and 43:41
the beliefs are this kind of filter that 43:44
affects what kind of memories we can 43:47
pull up and what kind of memories we 43:49
have in 43:51
>> what's the causal direction here right I 43:51
mean do we do we have memories and then 43:54
we form beliefs on the basis of those 43:57
memories or do we choose 44:00
beliefs and then selectively arrange our 44:03
memories so as to uh validate those 44:05
beliefs 44:09
uh my sense is yes 44:10
that is that it's like we build our 44:13
beliefs based on partly uh we build our 44:15
beliefs based on memories for things 44:19
that we've learned 44:20
>> but I think also based on things that 44:22
we've done um there's a literature on 44:24
what's called cognitive dissonance 44:27
that's very old and in in the cognitive 44:28
dissonance studies they used to have 44:31
people write essays on things that they 44:33
didn't actually believe that went 44:36
against their beliefs 44:37
And then later on they would their 44:39
beliefs would change to accommodate what 44:41
they did. It's kind of like the tail 44:44
wagging the dog, right? 44:45
>> Um and so that memory for what we've 44:47
done and what we've heard of off or you 44:51
know what loved ones, people we like and 44:54
care about have done will often have a 44:56
pull on what we believe later on. But of 44:59
course what we believe will affect the 45:01
kinds of memories we can access and the 45:04
way we remember them. Right? So uh just 45:07
as the simplest level people tend to 45:10
remember positive events more from their 45:12
lives than negative events. But more 45:15
importantly when they reconstruct them 45:17
they tend to remember them more 45:19
positively and they tend to remember 45:21
themselves more positively. Um, and that 45:22
can be good in some ways, but it can 45:26
also mean that you tend to think that 45:27
you're better than you really are, 45:29
right? And so now you magnify that to 45:31
your social group and you pull up 45:33
collective memories about, you know, the 45:35
past based on your beliefs. And then you 45:37
and then you construct a narrative out 45:41
of it. And maybe that narrative is that 45:42
our people are great. Maybe that 45:45
narrative is we used to be great and now 45:47
we're terrible. We need to be great 45:50
again. 45:51
Support for this show comes from Mint 45:54
Mobile. You know what feels great? 45:56
Saying yes to saying no. When both the 45:58
bachelorette party and the wedding are 46:00
destinations, you can say yes to saying 46:02
no. When the event starts after 10 p.m. 46:04
on a Tuesday, that's a yes or a no. And 46:06
maybe most importantly, if you're still 46:10
overpaying for wireless, yeah, no. At 46:11
Metmobile, their favorite word is no. No 46:15
contracts, no monthly bills, no 46:18
overages, no hidden fees. Ditch 46:19
overpriced wireless and their 46:21
jaw-dropping monthly bills, unexpected 46:23
overages, and hidden fees. Plans start 46:25
at $15 a month at Mint. All plans come 46:27
with high-speed data and unlimited talk 46:30
and text delivered on the nation's 46:32
largest 5G network. Use your own phone 46:34
with any MintMobile plan and bring your 46:37
phone number along with all your 46:39
existing contacts. Ready to say yes to 46:41
saying no? Make the switch at 46:43
mintmobile.com/gray 46:45
area. That's mintmobile.com/gray 46:47
area. Upfront payment of $45 required, 46:50
equivalent to $15 a month. Limited time 46:52
new customer offer for first three 46:55
months only. Speeds may slow above 35 46:57
gigabytes on unlimited plan. Taxes and 46:59
fees extra. See Mint Mobile for details. 47:02
Do we sometimes have to forget, let go 47:07
of memories in order to forgive and move 47:10
on, or is there another way to do that? 47:14
I'm really asking about this at both the 47:16
individual 47:17
>> and the social level, you know, we're 47:18
talking about politics and and tribal 47:20
identities and shared memories. Do you 47:23
think even at the level of society that 47:25
sometimes 47:27
we need to forget in order to move 47:29
forward or is the price of that kind of 47:31
amnesia too high? And I'm thinking of 47:32
something like, 47:35
>> you know, truth and reconciliation. Yes. 47:36
>> Commissions, right? Or even the battles 47:38
we have in this country uh over our 47:40
history. I mean, I've always believed 47:42
that we have to confront the truth of 47:44
our past uh before we can move beyond 47:47
it. But I guess I I can also understand 47:49
how someone might say, well, you know, 47:51
if we're constantly litigating the past 47:54
and re-remembering the past, we'll be 47:56
stuck there forever. 47:58
>> Yeah. And I I think the answer is 48:00
somewhere in between based on what we 48:03
know right now. And again, these are 48:05
there's serious limits to what we know. 48:06
But um I had a whole section in my book 48:08
that I wrote at one point on truth and 48:11
reconciliation uh uh um and restorative 48:13
justice and memory. And uh I had to 48:17
delete it because like my editor said, 48:21
this is just too controversial and uh 48:23
really not necessarily substantive 48:26
enough. And I think they were right that 48:28
the substance was a little bit low in 48:29
terms of the science. Um but um it's a 48:31
very controversial topic. Um you can 48:34
look at South Africa as a great example 48:36
of this. Um where these and this comes 48:38
back to what we talked about before with 48:43
trauma and this idea that you don't want 48:44
to forget but you want to remember it in 48:47
a way that's not radioactive. And uh so 48:49
a friend of mine named uh um uh Filipe 48:52
Dragard at Duke is doing this beautiful 48:56
beautiful project called the memory and 48:59
forgiveness project. And he's both a 49:01
philosopher and a cognitive 49:03
neuroscientist. And so he grew up in 49:04
Colombia and uh uh the time he grew up 49:07
in was just really tumultuous. There 49:11
were all these paramilitary groups and 49:13
government and uh um government uh um 49:15
crackdowns and uh uh insurgencies and so 49:19
forth and just many many many people 49:23
were killed. man traum the whole country 49:26
is still traumatized from this you know 49:29
um he has experiences of you know 49:31
harrowing you know things you know he 49:34
had a pretty privileged life compared to 49:36
a lot of people but just all sorts of 49:37
harrowing memories from so what he did 49:39
was he decided to bring in people into 49:42
communities and actually teach them 49:46
about memory and forgiveness and not 49:47
tell them you have to forgive people but 49:49
just give them the information and see 49:52
what they do with it and so the jury's 49:54
out. We don't know. Um, but what I love 49:56
from this is he has this quote from 50:00
somebody named Celia Cruz and the quote 50:02
is forgiving is not forgetting. 50:04
Forgiving is remembering without pain. M 50:07
>> and I think this is crucial because 50:10
often you have experiences like the 50:12
truth commission in South Africa and I 50:14
think where they went where it might 50:17
have been really making things worse for 50:19
people or at least that's what some 50:23
people have reported is they often felt 50:24
like it was just regurgitating the past 50:28
but not processing it. And people can 50:31
find this in psychotherapy too where you 50:34
you can have therapists who just dig and 50:37
dig and dig for trauma, but they don't 50:40
do much to kind of actually the the 50:44
assumption is always the truth will set 50:46
you free and if you remember something 50:48
that's traumatic somehow you'll just be 50:50
free of the psychopathology and that's 50:53
not at all true. I mean we like we've 50:55
talked about remembering something 50:58
horrible makes you feel horrible, right? 51:00
So unless you work to change the way you 51:02
think about the past, it's going to be 51:05
just kind of like retraumatizing people 51:08
over and over again. And I think that's 51:11
where forgiveness comes in is 51:13
forgiveness is changing the way you look 51:14
at the past. Not changing the memories 51:17
of what happened per se, but changing 51:20
your relationship with them. 51:22
What about self forgiveness 51:24
for someone who um 51:27
is stuck in the past or or cannot help 51:31
but ruminate over what they did wrong, 51:35
what they could have done, mistakes that 51:38
were made, etc., and and and 51:40
can't forgive themselves and sort of 51:44
move on. What do you think about that? 51:46
Do you have advice for people who are 51:48
stuck in that way? Well, I mean, I look 51:51
at I I I'm one of those people, so it's 51:54
really hard for me to give advice in the 51:56
sense of like telling people confidently 51:59
this is what you do. But 52:01
ultimately, 52:04
if we think of memory as this resource 52:05
as opposed to this repository of 52:08
absolute facts, right? 52:11
>> Then you can take the same information 52:13
from the past and make it useful or make 52:16
it, you know, counterproductive, right? 52:18
I mean, the past is literally over. It 52:21
does not exist anymore. At least, you 52:24
know, I mean, aside from physicists 52:26
telling you something about quantum 52:27
mechanics, you know, the past doesn't 52:28
exist anymore. Therefore, it's almost 52:30
like as if it's not real. All we're 52:33
living in is the present moment. So, to 52:36
the extent that we have this ability to 52:39
call on the past, the question is what 52:41
do we do with that? Right? If we see 52:43
that past as being something that's an 52:46
absolute marker of who we are and what 52:48
we're capable of, that's not going to be 52:51
productive. But you can look at the past 52:55
in a different way, right? I mean, I 52:57
talk about in the book just at a very 52:59
minor level that giving you yourself the 53:01
opportunity to make mistakes is the most 53:04
powerful way to learn, right? And self 53:06
forgiveness in some ways is really about 53:09
seeing the things that you've done that 53:13
were theoretically wrong and learning 53:15
from them. And so like yeah, you can 53:17
always look at mistakes you've made and 53:21
see them as learning experiences as part 53:23
of what it takes to, you know, learn and 53:26
grow. Or you can see them as, you know, 53:30
markers of how bad you are and how 53:33
incompetent you are. And obviously one's 53:35
more productive than the other. 53:37
>> I'm still beating myself up for stupid 53:39
I did in junior high. So 53:42
>> yeah, junior high lost cause. 53:44
>> Yeah, middle school for me was like 53:46
being in a state prison. So I definitely 53:48
get that idea. 53:51
>> Um, is there anything else 53:52
you want to say or any advice you want 53:56
to offer to anyone listening before we 53:58
ride off into the sunset? The point that 54:00
I really want people to get and I have 54:02
to hammer this home over and over again 54:05
is memor is not free. Memor is not easy 54:06
and memor is not absolute. And so the 54:10
first thing you have to do is set an 54:13
intention to what really is important 54:16
for you to remember. Whether it's going 54:19
on a family trip and you know deciding 54:21
do I want to take movies all the time or 54:24
do I want to be there and experience it 54:26
or being at a party and trying to 54:28
memorize people's names. It's all, you 54:30
know, the same kind of thing, which is 54:32
that your intention 54:34
will change what you remember and how 54:36
you remember it for the better. 54:39
>> Once again, the book is called Why We 54:40
Remember. It's fantastic, as was this 54:43
conversation. Thank you. 54:45
>> Thanks a lot for having me. It's been a 54:47
lot of fun. 54:49

– English Lyrics

📲 "" is trending – don’t miss the chance to learn it in the app!
By
Viewed
30,731
Language
Learn this song

Lyrics & Translation

[English]
Hey, I'm Sean, host of the Gray Area,
coming to you this week from a balcony
in sunny Denver, Colorado. For the show
this week, I talked to Charon Ranganoth.
He's a neuroscientist and author of the
book Why We Remember. It's one of my
favorite conversations we've had in a
while. It's a terrific book about how
memory works, why we remember what we
remember, and why we forget what we
forget, and how much of our memory is
actually just us making stuff up, and
how all of that shapes our identities as
individuals and also really as
societies. Anyway, it's terrific. I
think you'll love it. I did. And I'll
see you next week.
>> Dr. Shron Ragenoth, welcome to the show.
>> Thanks for having me, Sean. It's a
pleasure. Uh it's been too long since we
had a legit full-blown scientist on the
show. U
>> lots of pressure there.
>> Yeah, you got to carry the banner. Um
let's get into this wonderful book um
you wrote. I learned a lot. And
one of the things you say very early on
is that
the most important message to come out
of the whole science of memory isn't
that we should remember more, which is
what everyone just naturally wants to
do. You say that we actually have the
wrong expectation about what memory is
actually for. So I just wanted to start
there. What is memory for?
>> Well, I can tell you what it's not for.
And this is where the wrong expectation
comes in. It's not for storing and
hoarding every experience from the past.
It's about really giving us a resource
that we can draw upon to understand
what's happening right here and now and
to plan ahead and to anticipate what
could happen in the future.
>> So when
people say I have a bad memory, my
memory sucks, my memory is spotty. What
do you think they're actually
misunderstanding about how memory
functions?
>> Yeah, I mean it's a great question. I
have that voice in my head too that says
I have a terrible memory, right? But uh
I've been saying that for a long time
and I never struggled in school. Um I
could always remember things that I
needed to for work for instance. And it
wasn't so much that I had a bad memory
as much as I couldn't always remember
what I felt was important in the moment
when I needed it. And I think that's
what sometimes people feel. Uh but if
somebody really has a bad memory, like a
bad memory, they couldn't function in
life, they couldn't work, they couldn't
live independently. And I know cuz I've
I've tested people and I've I've
interviewed patients with bad really bad
clinically bad memories and they just
can't function. So I think often when I
see people complain about their memory,
they're often hyperfunctioning people
who just feel like they should be
remembering everything. And so that
that's what I'm talking about is I'm
really addressing those people.
So if a truly bad memory means you can't
function as an adult in the world, fair
enough. But clearly some memories are
better than others, right? Or or is the
mistake my mistake insisting on using
words like better or worse?
>> That's exactly the mistake is using
words like better or worse, right? So
it's often and I say that meaning that
people often conflate more with better.
So here here's a good example of this,
right? So there's people who have what's
called highly superior autobiographical
memory. So these are people who can go
back years into the past and recall from
a particular date like what they did for
what they had for breakfast or you know
who won a baseball game that day and
what the score was, the weather um that
day. Um all of these details that most
uh neurotypical people would not
remember, right? And so you'd think,
okay, these people have a great memory.
Uh but for one, they don't in the sense
that if you ask them to learn a new
language, they will probably learn at
the same speed as anyone who doesn't
have highly superior autobiographical
memory. Um based on the research that's
out there so far, there doesn't seem to
be some kind of a benefit that makes
them just better at everything. Um, the
second thing is we know if you look at
reports from people with highly superior
autobiographical memory, it's not like
they're like Bradley Cooper on
Limitless. They're not like uh
controlling the world. They're not any
more happy or successful than other
people as far as we can tell. And if
anything, they're a little bit plagued
by this memory because they often will
ruminate about things that happened,
even very minor things that happened
that were negative. And so, is that a
better memory? I mean, some people
online I've seen have described it as a
curse that they wouldn't wish on anyone.
>> Yeah. I mean, I don't want to be, you
know, Rainman or or Bradley Cooper here.
Um, but I'm sure I'm not alone.
>> Well, I wouldn't be I wouldn't mind
being Bradley Cooper for a day, but
I'm sure he has his own issues.
Everybody does, right?
>> Fair. Um, it would be fun to be more
handsome for a day. But sorry about
that. Uh my question would be why is it
that I can have near photographic
memories about certain things uh certain
books, certain events, other mostly
random stupid trivial stuff and have so
little capacity so often to remember
things like faces and names even though
I'd very much like to remember faces and
names. And I think there are very good
reasons, moral, uh, social, whatever, to
remember faces and names. And yet I just
consistently
botch it. U, why is that?
>> Well, there's a whole lot of reasons.
But, um, if we, uh, talk about the
problem of faces and names, this is
something that everyone who complains
about memory, this is the first thing
that comes up. So, you're not alone.
Essentially, when we're trying to pull
information from memory, we have a
challenge which is that memories compete
with each other. So any experience that
we have uh there's all sorts of
competition for it. And just as an
analogy, you know, my desk right now,
I'm trying to gather information for my
taxes cuz I postpone them and I spend a
good amount of time trying to find
things all the time, right? And so
memory can be thought of in a very
similar way that when you have a lot of
similar things cluttered around and not
organized, you're going to have a lot of
competition, a lot of trouble finding
it. But if there was one thing that
truly stood out here, like if I had a
violin amidst all these other things, I
would be able to find that pretty
easily, right? Um, so here's the problem
now getting back to faces and names is
we hear many many similar names.
Sometimes the same name attached to
multiple faces, right? And faces are
exceptionally similar to one another.
There's obviously your face is different
from mine, but we both have two eyes, we
have a nose, we have mouths. And even
amongst, you know, people we know well,
there's often a lot of similarity. Uh so
now we have a real problem is that we
have a bunch of faces that kind of are
similar to each other. There's a number
of names that are similar to one another
that we've seen in the past. And the
association between a name and a face is
just utterly ridiculous, right? I mean,
how do you there's nothing meaningful
linking a name to a face. So if you met
somebody whose name was Baker, you know,
hundreds of years ago, that would be
easier because at least they were
probably baking bread and you know, or
Smith would be the guy who's making your
suit of armor or your swords or
whatever, right? And so now there's just
no link between names and faces. There's
nothing meaningful there. So that's why
it's so hard.
>> How differently does memory function at
different stages of life? I think we all
understand that memory declines
uh as we get older, but there are
certain kinds of memory that seem to
hold up better than others as we age,
right? What is the reason for that?
>> Let's get into the different kinds of
memory first of all. So, um there's many
different kinds of memory. memory
researchers love to slice them and dice
them in different ways, but one that I
think is meaningful, especially when we
talk about development, is the
distinction between what's called
semantic memory and episodic memory. And
so semantic memory might be like, you
know, remembering uh something about,
let's say, like uh um you know, a
history lesson that you took when you
were in high school and remembering the
facts about World War II or being able
to build knowledge about like uh
computers or something like that and
that kind of expertise that you can
build up. Uh, but episodic memory is the
ability to link something to one
particular moment in time, like a movie
that you saw about World War II when you
were on a strange date when you were in
college or something and for some reason
you decided to see an old showing of
Midway or whatever it is, right? Um, and
those particular events that are unique
to a place in a time are called episodic
memories. And so when people say their
memory gets worse as they get older,
they're almost always talking about
episodic memory, which is that uh people
when you get older, you find yourself
more forgetful, misplacing things and
not being able to remember where they
were or not being able to remember faces
and names. Semantic memory actually
remains solid and even kind of improves
a little bit as people get older. um
that knowledge about the world. It's not
like when you're 70, you lose knowledge
about all the things that you've
learned, all that wisdom, so to speak,
right? So, what you do lose though is
the ability to pull up those facts
sometimes. And so, you'll end up being
like, I know the name of that actor, but
I can't get it. It's that guy who's in
that thing. And that happens a lot, too.
And that's not a issue of losing the
memory or not being able to form
semantic memories as much as it is a
problem of not being able to pull up
that information. And that has to do
with something else altogether which we
call executive function. And in fact,
that's actually probably one of the
biggest reasons why memory gets episodic
memory gets worse as you get older is
you lose that ability to regulate your
attention, that ability to pull up
strategies, that ability to kind of like
not just make memories, but make
memories useful. And that's related to
the functions of an area called the
prefrontal cortex. And the prefrontal
cortex's function starts to decline
pretty much from the age of 30. And it's
a gradual decline in most people that
continues throughout the lifespan.
>> I'm on the wrong side of that.
>> Yeah, me too. I always tell people the
biggest memory hack I can give is to
tell people what do you want? Tell ask
yourself what do I want to remember in
the first place? What's the important
thing that I need to take away from
this? If you came in to a cocktail party
and you knew you wanted to be a good
person and meet all, you know, get to
know the names of all the people you
meet and remember them, then start with
that intention. Most people think that
they'll get the memories for free and
that's just wrong. There are studies
showing that the happiness and
satisfaction we get from the outcomes of
our decisions do not come as you might
expect from what we actually experienced
but rather it comes from what we
remember.
That seems like a pretty significant
finding. What is the explanation for
that?
Well, memory really is so transient. And
what I mean by this is is that we don't
forget everything, right? But we forget
many of the details very, very quickly.
Some of the earliest studies of memory
showed that in fact within about one
hour of memorizing things that you would
lose about 60% of what you tried to
memorize.
>> Now, think about that. That's like 60%
of the details of your experiencing self
at this moment gone in an hour right
now. That doesn't mean that you don't
remember anything. But uh Danny Conaman
who won the Nobel Prize in economics, he
was actually said a lot of smart things
about memory. Like he just said smart
things about a lot of things, but one
was memory where he said people
generally remember the beginnings and
the endings and the highs and the lows.
And that in some ways is the important
stuff that you need for making a
decision, not necessarily every bit of
experience.
>> One takeaway for me here is is that the
stories we tell ourselves u the the
narratives that make up our memory
really matter in terms of our
well-being. And that would seem to mean
that we could gain more happiness in
life by deliberately focusing on the
right things by trying to remember the
right things. Um
>> I think that was a beautiful way of
putting it. Um I like to say that memory
is more like a painting than a
photograph. You know, there's a famous
painting by Van Go of this uh French
church, and I like to show this
sometimes when I'm talking to people uh
and people will immediately recognize
the painting as one of the church,
right? But it by no means is exactly
like the church that's there. I mean,
there's a lot of stuff that's could be
thought of as missing or distorted
relative to what's in the church itself,
what what it looks like, but there's a
lot that's just his perspective that's
in there. And that's what makes a
painting beautiful in some ways is stuff
that's neither accurate nor inaccurate,
but it's just how the person sees the
world. He painted it at night. You could
have just as easily painted it in the
morning. You could paint it from a
different point of view. You could paint
it small. You could paint it big. You
could look at one door, you know? And
every time we remember an event, we're
creating a new painting, right? So often
people will tell me about good and bad
memories, good and bad experiences, but
that can change. And I think even though
that seems a little bit
counterintuitive, I think many of us
have had times in our life which were
objectively
bad, sometimes terrible, and we can look
back on that from a completely different
point of view. So in my book I talk
about an experience which just like
almost everything that could go wrong
did go wrong when I was going on this
paddle boarding expedition
almost died you know in that thing and
now it's a pretty funny story I really
like to share it. Yeah. Well that that
story actually in the book made me think
about
trauma and memory and even worse kinds
of traumatic experiences than
>> than that. Right. Right. I mean, so for
someone who's been through uh real
trauma, uh just terrible, terrible
trauma, forgetting
can feel impossible. I mean, how do
traumatic memories differ from ordinary
memories in the brain? Do they differ at
all?
>> Oh, they do for sure. Um for one thing,
the brain is wired in a way to
preferentially keep memories for things
that are emotionally significant. And
the reason is our emotions are tied to
they're not equivalent to but they're
tied to these motivational circuits in
our brain that are trying to keep us
alive, right? And so um often when
people have a traumatic event, they feel
an exist existential threat and they're
often arousing or stressful. And there's
chemicals in our brain that are released
during these experiences.
Noradrenaline's one. Um cortisol is a
stress hormone. There's gobs of hormones
that are released during stress. Uh, but
there's also exciting kind of more
positive experiences like that kind of
moment when you're going to get your
first kiss or something like that and
that's associated with dopamine release
in the brain. There's all sorts of
chemicals like this. They promote
plasticity. They allow new memories to
be locked in much more effectively,
right? So, that's why these traumatic
memories tend to stick with us often.
Um, but what's interesting about it is
we've studied some stressful memories
and some emotionally significant
memories, not in ways that are remotely
like something that a trauma
survivor really experiences. But what we
find even with those is there's a
difference between the brain systems
that actually um are involved in giving
you the details of what happened and the
brain systems that actually just rev you
up and give you the visceral feeling of
the experience, right? So let's say you
got into a car accident. Later on you
remember the car accident. Well, there's
the details of what happened, right?
like this person ran a stop sign and
they crashed right into me and they're
driving like a red car. Um, and then
there's the feeling the feeling of
imminent danger, the feeling of pain
when you got hit and you you jerked
forward or something, right? And it
turns out that those are different, but
the thing that makes people feel like
it's a photographic memory or they can
experience it like in the moment is not
the details. It's really the emotion
that's associated with it. And so that's
a key thing that makes traumatic
memories different. And this brings me
to another point that I I like to bring
up to people who tell me, you know, can
I can you help me forget this
experience? You want to be able to
remember them without that emotional
punch in the face. Lots of people who do
survive traumas can do that. Um the
brain has natural ways. Matt Walker says
that it happens during sleep. And I
think that is a part of it that the
brain changes these memories to become
more useful to us and less toxic to us
later on, less radioactive.
But when it doesn't happen, that's when
people get PTSD. I
really enjoyed the bits in the book
about music and the unique power it has.
And and I thought I thought it was
fascinating when you were talking about
the study you worked on, I think when
you were uh just a you know a baby grad
student. Um and and you you expected
that having subjects listen to sad music
would make it easier for them to
remember sad events in their lives and
and that remembering sad events would
make them sad. But why does listening to
sad music make us remember sad events
and does the dynamic also work in the
opposite direction? I think this is a
very important point is that our
episodic memories are rooted to a place
in time and part of what makes a unique
time in our lives, a unique moment in
time significant is uh the feelings that
we have in the time. You might be
remembering something like, you know,
your high school graduation and there's
the content of that memory, but then
there's also the feelings that you had
at that moment. And that those feelings
are part of what we call the context.
And so if you imagine episodic memories,
if they were books in a library, you
would be sorting those books not
according to the content, but really to
the context.
And that as I said the emotions are part
of that context. And so if in the moment
you are in a particular emotional state
it makes it's almost like as if you're
looking at the section of the library
that has information relevant to that
context. And so uh that's why music can
be such a powerful vehicle. And it's not
only the emotional part. It also can be
because you listen to particular kinds
of music during particular times in your
life.
>> Yeah, that's it's such a unique trigger
in that way. I think you I mean you talk
in the book about, you know, if you hear
a couple of chords of born in the USA,
it reminds you of some of the racist
you would hear when you were in
like junior high or high school. And you
know, it it made me think if I hear the
the Rolling Stone song Wild Horses, it
takes me right back to when my mom died
in 2020. I listen to that song over and
over again uh when she passed away
because it reminded me of her. It was
her favorite song. And for the rest of
my life, if that song comes on, I am
right back there when that happened. I
mean, it's just I just there's nothing
else that quite does that in that way. I
think part of it is the emotional pull
and part of it is the uniqueness of
particular kinds of music or pieces of
music to particular kinds of
relationships we have or times in our
life. Who we are is constantly changing,
right? And music is often tied to our
sense of who we are that it's like we
choose particular times of music based
on who we are at a particular time in
our life. And so music is this beautiful
vehicle to access those memories of who
we were. And that's part of I think why
there's this nostalgia that's associated
with music is because it really takes us
back not only to the memories of what
happened, but the feeling of what we
were like when we were at that point in
our lives.
>> Support for the show comes from Bombas.
You know those songs that make you just
want to run? Maybe you're in the middle
of a workout and you're totally spent.
But then that one track hits, maybe it's
tulle, maybe it's a little wooang and
boom, you're back. That's kind of like
what Bombas socks do. You put them on
and you're ready to go. This summer, you
can get moving with Bombas socks.
Specially designed to wick sweat, keep
you cool, and prevent blisters. You
probably know I've tried out Bombas for
myself. They are my favorite socks. In
the winter and fall, I rock their wool
socks because I wear boots a lot and
they're super thick but still
breathable. Now that it's 500° down here
on the Gulf Coast, my go-to socks are
their lightweight athletic socks. They
are comfortable and they really do keep
you cool and dry. You can head over to
bombas.com/gra
area and use code grey area for 20% off
your first purchase. That's
boombas.com/gray
area codegray area at checkout.
Support for the show comes from Shopify.
When you're starting a new business, it
can feel like you're expected to do it
all. Marketing, design, and everything
in between, even if you've never done
half of it before. What you really need
is a tool that helps you reach your
goals without having to master every
skill yourself. For millions of
businesses, that tool is Shopify. Their
design studio lets you build a beautiful
online store that matches your brand
style, letting you choose from hundreds
of readytouse templates. You can also
set up your content creation by using
their helpful host of AI tools. And you
can even create email and social media
campaigns with ease and meet your
customers wherever they're scrolling or
strolling. See why Shopify is the
commerce platform behind millions of
businesses around the world. If you're
ready to sell, you can be ready with
Shopify. You can turn your big business
idea into a reality with Shopify on your
side. You can sign up for your $1 per
month trial period and start selling
today at shopify.com/box.
Go to shopify.com/box.
Shopify.com/box.
I wanted to ask you um about the role of
memory in shaping our identities, our
our sense of self. And you talk about
how our life narrative is really this
thing stitched together through memory
and our social interactions and the
stories we tell about who we are and
what we are. My question is why do we
need these stories in the first place? I
mean is the self or whatever we call the
self just a memory system holding a
story together?
>> I in some ways yes I think so. In fact
there are people who talk in my field
about the self-memory system. Uh, I in
the book, if there's one thing I'm
guilty of is avoiding some of the hard
questions, questions about consciousness
and the self.
>> Smart man.
>> Yeah, that was just a little bit of a
copout. I totally admit that. Um, but I
think you're on to something and I think
that it's like uh we have so I talk in
the book about schemas as a very
powerful way in which we can learn. So
schemas for instance could be something
as simple as I know what happens during
a basketball game. I know what happens
during a wedding. So these particular
kinds of events we have blueprints in
our head about things that are almost
certain to happen, things that could
happen and things that won't happen,
right? And that helps us orient
ourselves towards the unique aspects of
what um you're trying to remember at any
given time. uh but we also have those
ideas about oursel called a self schema.
And I think this is important because a
big part of imagination and planning is
understanding a little bit about who we
are and why we do things. We don't just
remember the past in terms of like
regurgitating a bunch of details. When
we're really remembering events, we
stitch it into causes and effects. And
that's really important because if
something is not a cause or an effect,
it's kind of window dressing, right? And
so we don't remember that somebody was
like, you know, left the room, but we
remember they left a room because they
were angry at us or because they were in
they just got interrupted by an
emergency phone call. And I think our
sense of who we are gives us a way of
organizing our past and organizing
things into causes and effects, too.
>> This is more of a philosophical question
than a scientific one, but this is the
show, so I'm going to ask anyway. Um,
if someone loses their memories,
if they cannot remember their life,
can they still be themselves? Can they
be the same self they were?
>> There's a uh there's a person named Stan
Klene at at UC Santa Barbara who asked
this very question and he makes a strong
case. I tend to think of philosophy as
above my pay grade.
There's a, you know, I tend to invert
the usual like kind of tech bro view of
the world and I kind of think of math
and physics as easy fields because
there's an answer and psychology is a
much more of a hard science because
there's not really clear answers and
philosophy is like where you get into
the stratosphere.
>> We just have to sound like we know what
we're talking about. You you people, the
scientists actually have to know what
>> Yeah. Yeah. Exactly. I think it's like
the real experts will say, "I don't
know." Um, and I don't know, but uh, uh,
Stan has studied people with very, very
dense amnesia and has made a good case
that these people still have a sense of
who they are.
>> But here's the thing. So there there's
not cases literally of people losing
every memory of what they've
experienced. But there are people, for
instance, like a patient named Casey
that Endel Tolving studied. He's he's a
really f he came up with the concept of
episodic memory. And the claim was he
could look all the way back in the past
and he still didn't have that ability to
mentally time travel, right? And there's
a little bit of an emptiness to them, a
little bit of a flatness. They're in the
moment, but there's not a sense of what
they could do or what they could be
because they don't have these episodic
memories to draw upon to give them like
kind of more of a flavor of who they
are. Um, and I think that's really
important because and like I said, our
sense of self normally is constantly
changing as we keep acquiring episodic
memories.
>> But people who have amnesia, even if
it's like something more like the
typical kind of amnesia is you have a
stroke or something and you're
unconscious for a while. when you come
to you lose memories for something. You
lose your ability to form new memories
and you might lose memories going back
10 years but you still remember your
childhood. And what happens is these
patients who have severe amnesia
will get stuck in time. You know,
they'll look in the mirror and they'll
say, "Oh my gosh, who is this person?"
But to them, it's like they're still
they still feel like they were the
person they were, you know, 20 years
ago, 30 years ago. And in some cases, I
think that's why people with severe
Alzheimer's, you sense that they're
regressing as they kind of like lose
that ability to go back to that
wellspring of episodic memories. So I
think yeah, we have a capability of
being having a self, but that self
becomes much emptier and thinner as you
lose memories.
>> Can we say that political movements in
some sense are doing the same thing just
at a
at another level. Are they stitching
collective memory into a collective
identity? 100% 100%. Uh this idea of
collective memory uh is uh still one
that we're is it's still a developing
concept in our field. A lot of the work
that's been done in collective memory is
actually done in the fields of history
and sociology. But it's a very relevant
concept. I mean just as an example
>> how would you define that if you don't
mind me asking just
>> absolutely I mean you can define it very
broadly which is a shared memory that a
memory that's shared amongst multiple
people if you have close family members
you probably have a collective memory of
the family and that gives you a sense of
your familial identity uh because you
all have this shared story right and
your experience might be different than
the experience of your parents or
siblings but once you share those
experiences with each other. It's no
longer your memory. Uh just the act of
expressing that memory in a way that
someone else can understand it changes
the memory for you. And then now the
other person has a memory for something
they've never experienced before because
you've told them about it. And so as
these memories go get passed back and
forth, they're constantly modified,
right? And I think this is key to what
makes humans so successful in the world
is that we can build memories and
benefit from other people's experiences
and and you know wisdom. But it creates
huge problems because memory can be
selective and memory can be you know so
it can be highly incomplete and it can
also be inaccurate many times. Right.
And so
>> sometimes intentionally. So
>> sometimes intentionally. Exactly. So if
you want to form an authoritarian
regime, the first thing you do is you
seize collective memory. You take down
statues. You start to regulate, you
know, the teaching of history in
schools. You remove references in
historical archives, right? Almost any
authoritarian regime. This is part of
the playbook. And uh and I think it's a
powerful one because once you start to
shape that narrative, just like people
have narratives of I'm terrible at math
or I'm like a failure in life and those
are often rooted in a particular
interpretation of your past. If you can
enforce most people to have a particular
view of their nation's past, that's
going to shape their sense of what it
means to be a member of that nation or
that society.
The uh relationship between individual
memory and collective memory or between
individual identity and collective
identity is so fascinating to me. I how
much of our sense of self is determined
or or shaped by our social
or tribal affiliations. I imagine the
relationship here is pretty damn
complicated, but there has to be a
relationship there.
>> There's absolutely a relationship and
it's a two-way relationship.
>> So, a lot of the research shows that on
the one hand, you develop your sense of
of uh tribal affiliations
or familial relationships through these
kind of collective memories that you
have. but also you're able to connect
with other people if you have shared
collective memories and you view those
memories in a similar light. And so what
I mean by that is, you know, if you're
in England and you support say the
Tottenham Spurs or something like that,
you can meet someone anywhere and once
you start talking about the same game,
they will become friends, right? Uh here
in the US it would be like an American
football team or a baseball team, same
thing, right? or let's say you're both
you both have similar political views
and you both like uh saw the same debate
or something like that. Just sharing
those collective memories can make you
feel more connected with that person.
>> Oh, I I grew up in the in the South.
That's where I am now. And you know,
like college football is sort of our
thing. It's a religion.
>> But it is people who who
>> didn't grow up in that don't quite
understand it at this level. what kind
of cultural shared cultural language it
is. I mean, it is it is that shared
memory really is you meet someone, they
got on an LSU Tigers shirt and before
you know it, you're having a
conversation about, oh no, I went there
when I was five. the Auburn game. Could
you believe, you know, I mean, it was
>> it it it has this way of of stitching
together a history and a community in a
way that that very few things can, but
it is that
>> it is that that shared cultural language
and history that's like the container
for the community really.
>> Yeah. It's beautiful in many ways and
it's toxic in many ways, right? It's
like
>> that's where I was going next.
>> Yeah. Yeah. I mean, it's both because
you see someone else with an LSU Tigers
thing shirt and now you feel like you've
got a bond with them and that's a really
good thing. But then you're both
watching an LSU Tigers game and you talk
to each other and you'll remember the
game based on your love for the LSU
Tigers and you'll go, "Yeah, the ref
totally blew that call." And you might
forget a really good play that was made
by the other team. and the other person
reinforces that story and you build a
collective memory that's more selective
and more distorted than it would be had
you never talked to each other. And
that's the downside of collective memory
is if we're not careful, if we don't
surround ourselves by a lot of checks
and balances, what happens is we can
develop very impoverished and inaccurate
collective memories and we're very
susceptible to misinformation because we
like misinformation if it comes in a
flavor that we already like and we're
much more likely to take it in and
digest it.
>> Yeah. Now, this is something
This is the challenge or one of the
challenge uh that comes from the fact
that memory it itself is so constructed
and slippery in this way. I mean you
even in the book you you talk a bit
about how as we grow more segregated
politically, culturally,
we end up with just totally different
memories
>> of the same events
>> and therefore different realities.
>> And if if memory is reconstructive in in
in the ways you're talking about and we
are moving more and more into a world
that's segregatedformationally.
Does that seem like a massive massive
political problem to you? I mean, I've
argued that it's just straight up
unsustainable, but it's also possible
that I'm being dramatic. Uh, so I put it
to you. I think this is one of the
biggest threats we have in the present
day and one of the biggest dangers that
I foresee as you know technologies like
AI become more and more powerful is you
know and maybe I'm I'm looking at this
from too much of a personal lens and too
much of a you know often we kind of look
at things like now is somehow special
right but I do feel like I've never
lived in a time where it's so easy for
people to just believe what makes them
happy, right? We used to all have shared
news sources and shared sources of
information and expertise that, you
know, they could tell us things that we
didn't like, like smoking is unhealthy.
And even if we didn't like that, we sort
of had to believe it because we were
all, you know, because we had news
sources that fact checked and we had
like uh uh experts who we trusted and
experts aren't always right, you know,
and news sources are definitely not
always right. And you know, news can be
a selective win uh lens on on collective
memory. But now we have so many uh
sources of information and the uh thing
that I of you know I've done a lot of
podcasts now and it's been a fascinating
experience for me because I hear you
know I remember going on one podcast and
and the person was talking about the
legacy media is dead and and they're
trying to go after podcasters because
podcasters are telling it like it is and
giving people these long form things.
And the idea is somehow if we hear
somebody talking for an hour and we just
let them say whatever they want that
eventually people will figure out the
truth, right? But that's not really how
it works. What happens is people pick
and choose what they will remember from
all of the podcasts they've listened to
>> based on what they believe. That's how
they're going to form that memory for
all those things that they've been
exposed to. And the more we allow
misinformation to thrive,
the more it likely it is that people can
just pick and choose what they get in a
way that makes them feel good. I brought
up this idea of causes as being really
important, causes and effects. And so we
often will decide about facts and we'll
make decisions about things in the world
based on our understanding of causes and
effects. But the way our brains do it is
kind of like making the best of bad
information. So for instance, it's like
if I were to hear about somebody who
gets a shot and they, you know, gets a
vaccination and they developed a severe
allergic reaction to it and they go into
aniflaxis, I will be more likely to
think that that per that vaccine causes
problems and is dangerous.
But I'm not hearing the stories about a
bunch of people who took the vaccine and
didn't get sick. I'm not hearing them
and because that's not an event. That's
not something that you can form a memory
for. It's something that didn't happen.
You know, hearing a statistic that you
know nine out of 10 people who took the
shot didn't get CO or whatever. That's
not really going to produce a vivid
memory that's going to influence you
nearly as much as this vivid memory of
somebody you even if it wasn't you just
somebody telling a story about this.
>> Do we understand how malleable
collective memory is? Is this something
that can evolve pretty quickly? If I can
tell myself a new story about myself
that transforms how I
>> experience the world or think about the
world and if you can do that as an
individual,
can a country do that? Can a community
do that?
Everything we know about collective
memory, which is not a lot, but
everything we know has really pointed to
the idea that collective memory just
magnifies the strengths and the
weaknesses of individual memory. And so
what I mean by that is we know that I
can give people misinformation after an
event has occurred. So you you know
Elizabeth Loftess did some really
beautiful work on this um where she
would show you know a slideshow of this
car accident and then the car accident
like somebody might have run through
like a stop sign and she'll say how fast
was it going when it passed the yield
sign and later on people might be more
likely to remember that there was a
yield sign there instead of a stop sign.
Right? Those are effects that you can
find in the lab for fairly mundane kind
of pieces of information. But now you're
in a group of people and you're
remembering that and one person
remembers something wrong, especially if
that person has kind of a position of
status. They're a little bit higher
status and they talk more. Um, which we
can all think of what kinds of groups
tend to do this, right? You know,
mansplating things and so forth. um
those people will have an undue
influence on how people remember the
event later on and that misinformation
can really thrive like a I mean they
talk about it as a social contagion
because it spreads like a virus. Um so
that selectivity and that malleability
of memory that we have as individuals
even fairly mundane experiences tends to
just magnify in groups. Uh but of course
you also have the power of belief and
the beliefs are this kind of filter that
affects what kind of memories we can
pull up and what kind of memories we
have in
>> what's the causal direction here right I
mean do we do we have memories and then
we form beliefs on the basis of those
memories or do we choose
beliefs and then selectively arrange our
memories so as to uh validate those
beliefs
uh my sense is yes
that is that it's like we build our
beliefs based on partly uh we build our
beliefs based on memories for things
that we've learned
>> but I think also based on things that
we've done um there's a literature on
what's called cognitive dissonance
that's very old and in in the cognitive
dissonance studies they used to have
people write essays on things that they
didn't actually believe that went
against their beliefs
And then later on they would their
beliefs would change to accommodate what
they did. It's kind of like the tail
wagging the dog, right?
>> Um and so that memory for what we've
done and what we've heard of off or you
know what loved ones, people we like and
care about have done will often have a
pull on what we believe later on. But of
course what we believe will affect the
kinds of memories we can access and the
way we remember them. Right? So uh just
as the simplest level people tend to
remember positive events more from their
lives than negative events. But more
importantly when they reconstruct them
they tend to remember them more
positively and they tend to remember
themselves more positively. Um, and that
can be good in some ways, but it can
also mean that you tend to think that
you're better than you really are,
right? And so now you magnify that to
your social group and you pull up
collective memories about, you know, the
past based on your beliefs. And then you
and then you construct a narrative out
of it. And maybe that narrative is that
our people are great. Maybe that
narrative is we used to be great and now
we're terrible. We need to be great
again.
Support for this show comes from Mint
Mobile. You know what feels great?
Saying yes to saying no. When both the
bachelorette party and the wedding are
destinations, you can say yes to saying
no. When the event starts after 10 p.m.
on a Tuesday, that's a yes or a no. And
maybe most importantly, if you're still
overpaying for wireless, yeah, no. At
Metmobile, their favorite word is no. No
contracts, no monthly bills, no
overages, no hidden fees. Ditch
overpriced wireless and their
jaw-dropping monthly bills, unexpected
overages, and hidden fees. Plans start
at $15 a month at Mint. All plans come
with high-speed data and unlimited talk
and text delivered on the nation's
largest 5G network. Use your own phone
with any MintMobile plan and bring your
phone number along with all your
existing contacts. Ready to say yes to
saying no? Make the switch at
mintmobile.com/gray
area. That's mintmobile.com/gray
area. Upfront payment of $45 required,
equivalent to $15 a month. Limited time
new customer offer for first three
months only. Speeds may slow above 35
gigabytes on unlimited plan. Taxes and
fees extra. See Mint Mobile for details.
Do we sometimes have to forget, let go
of memories in order to forgive and move
on, or is there another way to do that?
I'm really asking about this at both the
individual
>> and the social level, you know, we're
talking about politics and and tribal
identities and shared memories. Do you
think even at the level of society that
sometimes
we need to forget in order to move
forward or is the price of that kind of
amnesia too high? And I'm thinking of
something like,
>> you know, truth and reconciliation. Yes.
>> Commissions, right? Or even the battles
we have in this country uh over our
history. I mean, I've always believed
that we have to confront the truth of
our past uh before we can move beyond
it. But I guess I I can also understand
how someone might say, well, you know,
if we're constantly litigating the past
and re-remembering the past, we'll be
stuck there forever.
>> Yeah. And I I think the answer is
somewhere in between based on what we
know right now. And again, these are
there's serious limits to what we know.
But um I had a whole section in my book
that I wrote at one point on truth and
reconciliation uh uh um and restorative
justice and memory. And uh I had to
delete it because like my editor said,
this is just too controversial and uh
really not necessarily substantive
enough. And I think they were right that
the substance was a little bit low in
terms of the science. Um but um it's a
very controversial topic. Um you can
look at South Africa as a great example
of this. Um where these and this comes
back to what we talked about before with
trauma and this idea that you don't want
to forget but you want to remember it in
a way that's not radioactive. And uh so
a friend of mine named uh um uh Filipe
Dragard at Duke is doing this beautiful
beautiful project called the memory and
forgiveness project. And he's both a
philosopher and a cognitive
neuroscientist. And so he grew up in
Colombia and uh uh the time he grew up
in was just really tumultuous. There
were all these paramilitary groups and
government and uh um government uh um
crackdowns and uh uh insurgencies and so
forth and just many many many people
were killed. man traum the whole country
is still traumatized from this you know
um he has experiences of you know
harrowing you know things you know he
had a pretty privileged life compared to
a lot of people but just all sorts of
harrowing memories from so what he did
was he decided to bring in people into
communities and actually teach them
about memory and forgiveness and not
tell them you have to forgive people but
just give them the information and see
what they do with it and so the jury's
out. We don't know. Um, but what I love
from this is he has this quote from
somebody named Celia Cruz and the quote
is forgiving is not forgetting.
Forgiving is remembering without pain. M
>> and I think this is crucial because
often you have experiences like the
truth commission in South Africa and I
think where they went where it might
have been really making things worse for
people or at least that's what some
people have reported is they often felt
like it was just regurgitating the past
but not processing it. And people can
find this in psychotherapy too where you
you can have therapists who just dig and
dig and dig for trauma, but they don't
do much to kind of actually the the
assumption is always the truth will set
you free and if you remember something
that's traumatic somehow you'll just be
free of the psychopathology and that's
not at all true. I mean we like we've
talked about remembering something
horrible makes you feel horrible, right?
So unless you work to change the way you
think about the past, it's going to be
just kind of like retraumatizing people
over and over again. And I think that's
where forgiveness comes in is
forgiveness is changing the way you look
at the past. Not changing the memories
of what happened per se, but changing
your relationship with them.
What about self forgiveness
for someone who um
is stuck in the past or or cannot help
but ruminate over what they did wrong,
what they could have done, mistakes that
were made, etc., and and and
can't forgive themselves and sort of
move on. What do you think about that?
Do you have advice for people who are
stuck in that way? Well, I mean, I look
at I I I'm one of those people, so it's
really hard for me to give advice in the
sense of like telling people confidently
this is what you do. But
ultimately,
if we think of memory as this resource
as opposed to this repository of
absolute facts, right?
>> Then you can take the same information
from the past and make it useful or make
it, you know, counterproductive, right?
I mean, the past is literally over. It
does not exist anymore. At least, you
know, I mean, aside from physicists
telling you something about quantum
mechanics, you know, the past doesn't
exist anymore. Therefore, it's almost
like as if it's not real. All we're
living in is the present moment. So, to
the extent that we have this ability to
call on the past, the question is what
do we do with that? Right? If we see
that past as being something that's an
absolute marker of who we are and what
we're capable of, that's not going to be
productive. But you can look at the past
in a different way, right? I mean, I
talk about in the book just at a very
minor level that giving you yourself the
opportunity to make mistakes is the most
powerful way to learn, right? And self
forgiveness in some ways is really about
seeing the things that you've done that
were theoretically wrong and learning
from them. And so like yeah, you can
always look at mistakes you've made and
see them as learning experiences as part
of what it takes to, you know, learn and
grow. Or you can see them as, you know,
markers of how bad you are and how
incompetent you are. And obviously one's
more productive than the other.
>> I'm still beating myself up for stupid
I did in junior high. So
>> yeah, junior high lost cause.
>> Yeah, middle school for me was like
being in a state prison. So I definitely
get that idea.
>> Um, is there anything else
you want to say or any advice you want
to offer to anyone listening before we
ride off into the sunset? The point that
I really want people to get and I have
to hammer this home over and over again
is memor is not free. Memor is not easy
and memor is not absolute. And so the
first thing you have to do is set an
intention to what really is important
for you to remember. Whether it's going
on a family trip and you know deciding
do I want to take movies all the time or
do I want to be there and experience it
or being at a party and trying to
memorize people's names. It's all, you
know, the same kind of thing, which is
that your intention
will change what you remember and how
you remember it for the better.
>> Once again, the book is called Why We
Remember. It's fantastic, as was this
conversation. Thank you.
>> Thanks a lot for having me. It's been a
lot of fun.

Key Vocabulary

Start Practicing
Vocabulary Meanings

memory

/ˈmɛməri/

A2
  • noun
  • - the mental capacity or faculty of retaining and reviving facts, events, impressions, etc., or of recalling or recognizing previous experiences

remember

/rɪˈmɛmbər/

A1
  • verb
  • - to bring back (a fact, event, or situation) to the mind; recall to mind

experience

/ɪkˈspɪəriəns/

B1
  • noun
  • - the knowledge or skill from doing or seeing things
  • noun
  • - a particular incident or event that you have been involved in
  • verb
  • - to have knowledge or skill from doing or seeing things

identity

/aɪˈdɛntɪti/

B2
  • noun
  • - the distinguishing character or personality of an individual

function

/ˈfʌŋkʃən/

B1
  • noun
  • - a special activity or purpose
  • verb
  • - to work or operate

recall

/rɪˈkɔːl/

B2
  • verb
  • - remember something from the past
  • noun
  • - the ability to remember facts and events

traumatic

/traʊˈmætɪk/

C1
  • adjective
  • - relating to or caused by a trauma

emotion

/ɪˈmoʊʃən/

B1
  • noun
  • - a natural instinctive state of mind deriving from one's circumstances, mood, or relationships with others

narrative

/ˈnærətɪv/

C1
  • noun
  • - a spoken or written account of connected events
  • adjective
  • - in the form of a story

forget

/fərˈɡɛt/

A1
  • verb
  • - fail to remember

individual

/ˌɪndɪˈvɪdʒuəl/

B1
  • noun
  • - a single human being
  • adjective
  • - single; separate

society

/səˈsaɪəti/

B1
  • noun
  • - the aggregate of people living together in a more or less ordered community

plasticity

/plæˈstɪsɪti/

C1
  • noun
  • - the capacity to be molded, altered, or changed

superior

/suːˈpɪriər/

B2
  • adjective
  • - higher in rank, status, or quality

episodic

/ɪˌpɪˈsɒdɪk/

C1
  • adjective
  • - occurring in episodes; intermittent

amnesia

/æmˈniːziə/

C1
  • noun
  • - loss of memory, usually resulting from shock, brain injury, etc.

forgive

/fərˈɡɪv/

A2
  • verb
  • - cease to feel resentment against

stress

/strɛs/

B1
  • noun
  • - pressure or tension exerted on a material object
  • noun
  • - a state of mental or emotional strain
  • verb
  • - to subject to pressure or strain

intention

/ɪnˈtɛnʃən/

B2
  • noun
  • - a thing intended; an aim or plan

useful

/ˈjuːsfəl/

A2
  • adjective
  • - able to be used for a practical purpose

selective

/sɪˈlɛktɪv/

B2
  • adjective
  • - of or characterized by selection; discriminating or exclusive

“memory, remember, experience” – got them all figured out?

⚡ Dive into vocabulary challenges in the app and lock in your knowledge right after jamming to ""

Key Grammar Structures

Coming Soon!

We're updating this section. Stay tuned!

Related Songs